"How Do We Know What We Know?"
At the moment there is a huge argument going on about the cause
of AIDS. Most people in this country -- but by no means all --
believe that the disease is caused by a virus known as HIV, the
Human Immunodeficiency Virus. In Africa, however, heads of
governments have flatly stated that they don't accept this. They
blame a variety of other factors, from diet to climate to genetic
disposition.
The available scientific evidence ought to be the same for
everyone. So how can there be such vast differences in what
people believe?
Part of the reason is what we might call the "Clever Hans"
effect. Clever Hans was a horse who lived in Germany early in
the twentieth century, and he seemed to be smarter than many of
the humans around him. He could answer arithmetic problems by
tapping out the correct answers with a fore-hoof, and give yes or
no answer to other questions -- Is London the capital of France?
-- by shaking or nodding his head, just like a human.
His owner, a respected Berliner named Wilhelm von Osten, was as
astonished as anyone by Clever Hans' abilities. There seemed no
way that he would commit fraud, particularly since Clever Hans
could often provide correct answers when von Osten was out of the
room, or even in a different town. The Prussian Academy of
Sciences sent an investigating committee, and they too were at
first amazed by the horse's powers. True, there were
inconsistencies in the level of performance, but those could
often be explained away.
Finally, almost reluctantly, the truth was discovered. Clever
Hans could not do arithmetic, and did not know geography and
history. He was responding to the body language of the audience.
Most observers, including members of the investigating committee,
wanted Hans to get the right answers. So they would
instinctively tense at the question, and relax when Hans gave the
right answer. The body movements were very subtle, but not too
subtle for Hans. He really was clever -- clever at reading non-
verbal cues from the humans around him.
We are no different from the groups who met Clever Hans. We all
want certain answers to be true. Given a mass of evidence, we
tend to notice the facts that agree with our preferences, while
explaining away the inconvenient ones that would tell us
otherwise. And AIDS is a disease so complex and so widespread
that you can find what appear to be exceptions to any general
rules about its cause, spread, or inevitable effects.
That, however, is only half the story. The other reason there
can be such intense arguments about AIDS applies equally well to
half the things -- or maybe today its ninety-nine percent of the
things -- in our lives. We have actual experience in certain
areas: boiling water hurts; you can jump off a ten-foot ladder
but you can't jump back up; the moon will be full about once a
month; it's colder in winter than in summer; coffee with salt
instead of sugar tastes terrible.
But there are a million other things in everyday life for which
we have no direct experience and explanation. Can you tell me
how a digital watch works? Why is a tetanus shot effective for
ten years, while even with an annual flu shot you are still
likely to get the flu? What does that computer of yours do when
you switch it on? How does e-mail from your computer travel
across the country to a friend on the opposite coast, or halfway
around the world? Just what is plastic, and how is it made? How
does your refrigerator work? When you flip a light switch, where
does the electricity come from? It's not like turning on a
faucet, where we know that somewhere a huge reservoir of water
sits waiting to be tapped. So how come the electricity is there
just when you need it?
I can give answers to these, in a hand-waving sort of fashion,
but if I want any sort of details I have to go and ask questions
of specialists whom I trust. And most of the questions that I've
just asked are not new, or even close to new. The refrigerator
was patented in 1834. The first plastics, like our electricity
supply, go back to the beginning of the twentieth century.
Good answers are available to every one of my questions, all we
have to do is seek them out. But what about the newer areas of
research, for which AIDS forms a fine example? When the experts
themselves are still groping their way toward understanding, and
still disagreeing with each other, what chance do the rest of us
have?
Not much, provided that we insist on direct evidence. Every one
of us must decide for ourselves who and what to believe. We,
like the audience of Clever Hans, are going to believe what we
want to believe until evidence to the contrary becomes awfully
strong.
And maybe even after that. We, as ornery humans, tend to go on
believing what we prefer to believe.
Copyright-Dr. Charles Sheffield-2000
"Borderlands of Science" is syndicated by:
|